无水奶油是什么| 善莫大焉什么意思| 白萝卜煮水喝有什么功效| 什么病会引起恶心| hpv检查是什么| 二甲双胍缓释片什么时候吃| 积食吃什么食物帮助消化| 喝豆浆有什么好处| 子宫肌层回声欠均匀是什么意思| 学五行属什么| 孕期心情不好对胎儿有什么影响| 心脏支架和搭桥有什么区别| 三朵花代表什么意思| 中元节开什么生肖| 懋是什么意思| 婴儿黄疸高有什么影响| 什么是马上风| 最近老做噩梦是什么原因| 食管反流吃什么药| 月经血黑是什么原因| 一个口一个犬读什么| 知了猴什么时候出土| dvf是什么档次的牌子| 怀孕后为什么会生化| 吃什么补肝血| 一什么不什么四字成语| 童子是什么意思| 放屁臭什么原因| 脂蛋白a高是什么意思| 水果有什么| 梦见莲藕是什么意思| 鼻翼两侧发红是什么原因| 血管堵塞有什么办法可以疏通| 下面干涩是什么原因导致的| 抄送和密送是什么意思| 孔子的原名叫什么| m0是什么意思| 光影什么| 嘴唇发紫是什么原因| bmi指数是什么意思| 八月一号什么星座| 嘌呤是什么物质| kenzo是什么牌子| 后脑勺发麻是什么原因| 蚝油是干什么用的| 细胞核由什么组成| 舌头两边锯齿状是什么原因| 阿莫西林治什么| 灶性肠化是什么意思| 肾结石可以吃什么| 99年属什么| 5月3号是什么星座| 腿肿是什么原因引起的怎么办| vsop是什么意思| 智商125是什么水平| 吃黄精有什么好处| 老人流口水是什么原因引起的| 上将相当于什么级别| 水命和什么命最配| 黑眼圈严重是什么原因| 侵犯是什么意思| 外包什么意思| 梦见打苍蝇是什么意思| 眼睛痛吃什么药好得快| 梦见被追杀预示什么| 胃气不足吃什么中成药| 眉头长痘痘什么原因| 椰子和椰青有什么区别| 梦见自己怀孕是什么意思| 色彩斑斓是什么意思| 属鸡的贵人是什么属相| 拔完牙吃什么药| 伤口消毒用什么好| rpa是什么| 寒湿吃什么药| 尿酸高是什么造成的| 扳机点是什么意思| 生活是什么| 俊俏是什么意思| 夏天喝什么水最好| 省纪委副书记是什么级别| 年轻人白头发是什么原因引起的| 碱是什么| 猫咪为什么害怕黄瓜| 胎盘宫底后壁是什么意思| 毛遂自荐是什么意思| 济南为什么叫泉城| xo酱是什么酱| 银杏叶片有什么作用| 腰部凉凉的是什么原因| 看结石挂什么科室| 肾炎是什么原因引起的| 仪轨是什么意思| 为什么要拔掉智齿| 大地鱼是什么鱼| 甘油三酯高是什么意思| 为什么喝牛奶会拉肚子| 甄嬛传什么时候上映的| 心绞痛是什么症状| 17数字代表什么意思| 万艾可是什么| ca199是什么检查项目| 口角炎用什么药膏| 肺部真菌感染用什么药最好| 淋巴结节吃什么药最好| 木危读什么| 水果什么时间段吃最好| 什么叫临床医学| gold是什么牌子| 8023是什么意思啊| 脑梗是什么意思| 宫颈纳氏囊肿是什么| 身上长红点很痒是什么原因| 看日历是什么生肖| 啦啦是什么意思| 缓解紧张吃什么药| 吃紫菜有什么好处和坏处| 手指脱皮是什么原因引起的| 杨新鸣包贝尔什么关系| 红艳煞是什么意思| 大条是什么意思| fujixerox是什么牌子| 开屏什么意思| 什么有什么造句| 中招是什么意思| 6月初9是什么日子| 左下腹疼挂什么科| 青蛙怕什么| 配子是什么| 肥牛是什么肉| 草莓什么季节种植| ca代表什么病| 金色搭配什么颜色好看| hdr是什么拍照功能| loewe是什么牌子| 罗红霉素和红霉素有什么区别| 孕妇做唐筛是检查什么| 睡觉尿多是什么原因| a股是什么| 六爻是什么意思| 咽口水喉咙痛吃什么药| 床上什么虫子夜间咬人| 喝酒后头疼吃什么药| 糖尿病吃什么水果好| 一失足成千古恨是什么意思| 检查前列腺需要做什么检查| 政协主席什么级别| 非洲人吃什么主食| 海明威为什么自杀| 偶尔是什么意思| 喝蜂蜜水对身体有什么好处| 脚气是什么菌| 天池为什么没有鱼| 做梦牙齿掉了是什么预兆| 无极是什么意思| 水对什么| 一什么老虎| bart是什么意思| hcg是什么意思| 马镫什么时候发明的| 有痰是什么原因| 暴殄天物是什么生肖| 42是什么生肖| 意难平是什么意思| 小孩掉头发是什么原因引起的| 心脏支架是什么病| 秀才指什么生肖| 明矾有什么作用| 什么可以醒酒| 实蛋是什么| 喝酒手麻是什么原因| 舌头烧灼感是什么原因| 淡竹叶有什么功效| 强迫思维是什么| 湿气重是什么原因引起的| 巴利属于什么档次的| 甲亢吃什么食物好| 白葡萄酒配什么食物| 吃什么可以修复子宫内膜| hm是什么牌子的衣服| 怀孕20天有什么症状| 骨龄偏小意味着什么| 农历正月是什么星座| 血小板升高是什么原因| 莘莘学子什么意思| 什么赴什么继| 瘤变是什么意思| 胃不舒服能吃什么水果| 什么是性侵| 美帝什么意思| 蜂蜜和什么不能一起吃| 什么是晶体| 什么水果含铁量最高| 肾炎的饮食应注意什么| 怀不上孕是什么原因造成的| 寂寞的反义词是什么| 牛肉排酸是什么意思| 血压低会导致什么后果| 碳酸氢钠有什么作用| 嘴巴里起泡是什么原因| 什么是气胸| 什么时候洗头是最佳时间| 拔完牙能吃什么| 眼皮有点肿是什么原因| 三加一是什么意思| 什么泡面最好吃| 男人吃什么增大增长| 梦见干活是什么意思| 结婚9年是什么婚| 白羊座男和什么星座最配| 南宁晚上有什么好玩的地方| 即日是什么意思| 高血压属于什么科| 透疹是什么意思| 安是什么单位| 冰妹什么意思| 9月28日什么星座| 甲流吃什么药| 孕妇吃红薯对胎儿有什么好处| 什么地飞翔| 什么有作为| 毛囊炎的症状是什么原因引起的| 莫逆是什么意思| 疮疖是什么样子图片| 起风疹了用什么快速方法能解决| 铁窗泪什么意思| china的形容词是什么| 女人的胸部长什么样| 甘油三酯指什么| 蕈是什么意思| 祖师香是什么意思| 食物链是什么意思| 黑豆有什么作用| 小鸟吃什么食物| 牙龈发炎吃什么消炎药| 口大是什么字| pmid是什么意思| 新生儿干呕是什么原因| 凡士林是什么| 人为什么要喝水| 一命呜呼是什么意思| shit什么意思中文| 什么样的闪电| 中午吃什么饭家常菜| 巨蟹座跟什么星座最配| 薄如蝉翼是什么意思| mds是什么病的简称| 大腿骨叫什么骨| 肝小钙化灶是什么意思| 环切手术是什么| 疥疮用什么药膏好得快| 血糖高是什么意思| 贵人是什么意思| 前列腺炎不治疗有什么后果| 束在什么情况下读su| 9月18日是什么日子| 新加坡什么工作最挣钱| 骨刺吃什么药| 521代表什么含义| 正事是什么意思| 怪是什么意思| 三十六计最后一计是什么| 遗传物质是什么| fzl什么意思| 百度Jump to content

12岁女孩放学路上落水 六旬老人救人后不幸遇难

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
百度 据统计,近十年武汉大学樱花平均初放日为3月13日。

Theory of mind in animals is an extension to non-human animals of the philosophical and psychological concept of theory of mind (ToM), sometimes known as mentalisation or mind-reading. It involves an inquiry into whether non-human animals have the ability to attribute mental states (such as intention, desires, pretending, knowledge) to themselves and others, including recognition that others have mental states that are different from their own.[1][2][3] To investigate this issue experimentally, researchers place non-human animals in situations where their resulting behavior can be interpreted as supporting ToM or not.

The existence of theory of mind in non-human animals is controversial. On the one hand, one hypothesis proposes that some non-human animals have complex cognitive processes which allow them to attribute mental states to other individuals, sometimes called "mind-reading" while another proposes that non-human animals lack these skills and depend on more simple learning processes such as associative learning;[4] or in other words, they are simply behaviour-reading.

Several studies have been designed specifically to test whether non-human animals possess theory of mind by using interspecific or intraspecific communication. Several taxa have been tested including primates, birds and canines. Positive results have been found; however, these are often qualified as showing only low-grade ToM, or rejected as not convincing by other researchers.

History and development

[edit]
Much of the early work on ToM in animals focused on the understanding chimpanzees have of human knowledge

The term "theory of mind" was originally proposed by Premack and Woodruff in 1978.[2][5] Early studies focused almost entirely on studying if chimpanzees could understand the knowledge of humans. This approach turned out not to be particularly fruitful and 20 years later, Heyes, reviewing all the extant data, observed that there had been "no substantial progress" in the subject area.[6]

A 2000 paper[7] approached the issue differently by examining competitive foraging behaviour between primates of the same species (conspecifics). This led to the rather limited conclusion that "chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see".[8]

In 2007, Penn and Povinelli wrote "there is still little consensus on whether or not nonhuman animals understand anything about unobservable mental states or even what it would mean for a non-verbal animal to understand the concept of a 'mental state'." They went on further to suggest that ToM was "any cognitive system, whether theory-like or not, that predicts or explains the behaviour of another agent by postulating that unobservable inner states particular to the cognitive perspective of that agent causally modulate that agent's behaviour".[9]

In 2010, an article in Scientific American acknowledged that dogs are considerably better at using social direction cues (e.g. pointing by humans) than are chimpanzees.[10] In the same year, Towner wrote, "the issue may have evolved beyond whether or not there is theory of mind in non-human primates to a more sophisticated appreciation that the concept of mind has many facets and some of these may exist in non-human primates while others may not."[5] Horowitz, working with dogs, agreed.[11]

In 2013, Whiten reviewed the literature and concluded that regarding the question "Are chimpanzees truly mentalists, like we are?", he stated he could not offer an affirmative or negative answer.[8] A similarly equivocal view was stated in 2014 by Brauer, who suggested that many previous experiments on ToM could be explained by the animals possessing other abilities. They went on further to make reference to several authors who suggest it is pointless to ask a "yes or no" question, rather, it makes more sense to ask which psychological states animals understand and to what extent.[12] At the same time, it was suggested that a "minimal theory of mind" may be "what enables those with limited cognitive resources or little conceptual sophistication, such as infants, chimpanzees, scrub-jays and human adults under load, to track others' perceptions, knowledge states and beliefs."[13]

In 2015, Cecilia Heyes, Professor of Psychology at the University of Oxford, wrote about research on ToM, "Since that time [2000], many enthusiasts have become sceptics, empirical methods have become more limited, and it is no longer clear what research on animal mindreading is trying to find" and "However, after some 35 years of research on mindreading in animals, there is still nothing resembling a consensus about whether any animal can ascribe any mental state" (Heyes' emphasis). Heyes further suggested that "In combination with the use of inanimate control stimuli, species that are unlikely to be capable of mindreading, and the 'goggles method' [see below], these approaches could restore both vigour and rigour to research on animal mindreading."[1]

Methods

[edit]

Specific categories of behaviour are sometimes used as evidence of animal ToM, including imitation, self-recognition, social relationships, deception, role-taking (empathy), perspective-taking, teaching and co-operation,[5] however, this approach has been criticised.[6] Some researchers focus on animals' understanding of intention, gaze, perspective, or knowledge, i.e. what another being has seen. Several experimental methods have been developed which are widely used or suggested as appropriate tests for nonhuman animals possessing ToM. Some studies look at communication between individuals of the same species (intraspecific) whereas others investigate behaviour between individuals of different species (interspecific).

Knower-Guesser

[edit]

The Knower-Guesser method has been used in many studies relating to animal ToM.[6]

Competitive feeding paradigm

[edit]

The competitive feeding paradigm approach is considered by some as evidence that animals have some understanding of the relationship between "seeing" and "knowing".[1]

Goggles Method

[edit]

In one suggested protocol, chimpanzees are given first-hand experience of wearing two mirrored visors. One of the visors is transparent whereas the other is not. The visors themselves are of markedly different colours or shapes. During the subsequent test session, the chimpanzees are given the opportunity to use their species-typical begging behaviour to request food from one of the two humans, one wearing the transparent visor and the other wearing the opaque. If chimpanzees possess ToM, it would be expected they would beg more often from the human wearing the transparent visor.

False Belief Test

[edit]

A method used to test ToM in human children has been adapted for testing non-human animals. The basis of the test is to track the gaze of the animal. One human hides an object in view of a second human who then leaves the room. The object is then removed.[14]

In nonhuman primates

[edit]

Many ToM studies have used nonhuman primates (NHPs). One study that examined the understanding of intention in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children showed that all three species understood the difference between accidental and intentional acts.[15]

Chimpanzees

[edit]

There is controversy over the interpretation of evidence purporting to show ToM in chimpanzees.[16]

Attribution of perception

[edit]

Chimpanzees were unable to follow a human's gaze to find food hidden under opaque bowls, but were able to do so when food was hidden in tubes that the experimenter was able to look into. This seems to suggest that chimpanzees can infer another individual's perception depending on the clarity of the mechanism through which the individual has gained that knowledge.[17]

Attempts to use the "Goggles Method" (see above) on highly human-enculturated chimpanzees failed to demonstrate they possess ToM.[9]

In contrast, chimpanzees use the gaze of other chimpanzees to gain information about whether food is accessible.[7] Subordinate chimpanzees are able to use the knowledge state of dominant chimpanzees to determine which container has hidden food.[18]

Attribution of intentions

[edit]

Young chimpanzees were shown to reliably help researchers perform tasks that involved reaching (such as picking up dropped items that the researcher struggled to retrieve), without specific prompting. This suggests that these chimpanzees were able to understand the researcher's intentions in these cases and acted upon them.[19][20]

In a similar study, chimps were provided with a preference box with two compartments, one containing a picture of food, the other containing a picture of nothing. Neither were actually related to the contents of the box. In a foraging competition game, chimpanzees avoided the chamber with the picture of food when their competitor had chosen one of the chambers before them.[21][22]

Captive bonobos such as Kanzi have been reported to show concern for their handlers’ well-being.[23] Bonobos also console other bonobos who are victims of aggressive conflicts and reconcile after participating in these conflicts.[24] Both of these behaviors suggest some semblance of ToM through an attribution of mental states to another individual.

Attribution of False Belief

[edit]

Chimpanzees have passed the False Belief Test (see above) involving anticipating the gaze of humans when objects have been removed. Infrared eye-tracking showed that the chimpanzee subjects’ gaze were focused on where the experimenter would falsely believe the object /subject to be, rather than focusing on its actual location of which the chimps were aware. This seems to suggest that the chimpanzees were capable of ascribing false belief to the experimenter.[25]

Evidence for ToM Present in Chimps? Present in Bonobos? References
Attribution to Perception/Knowledge
Using human gaze to find food hidden under opaque bowl No Call et al., 1998
Using human gaze to find food hidden in tube open to human Yes Call et al., 1998
Remembering what human had seen and inferring target of attention Yes Yes MacLean & Hare, 2012
Attribution to Intention
Helping humans retrieve objects when reaching Yes Warneken & Tomasello, 2008
Consoling victims of conflict and reconciling after participating in these conflicts Yes Clay & Waal, 2013
Attribution of False Belief
Anticipating that human will look in wrong location based on false belief Yes Yes Krupenye et al., 2016

Other primates

[edit]
Rhesus macaques selectively steal grapes from humans who are incapable of seeing the grape compared to humans who can see the grape.

In one approach testing monkeys, rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) are able to "steal" a contested grape from one of two human competitors. In six experiments, the macaques selectively stole the grape from a human who was incapable of seeing the grape, rather than from the human who was visually aware.[26] Similarly, free ranging rhesus macaques preferentially choose to steal food items from locations where they can be less easily observed by humans, or where they will make less noise.[citation needed]

The authors also reported that at least one individual of each of the species showed (weak) evidence of ToM.[27]

In a multi-species study, it was shown that chimpanzees, bonobos and orangutans passed the False Belief Test (see above).[25]

In 2009, a summary of the ToM research, particularly emphasising an extensive comparison of humans, chimpanzees and orang-utans,[28] concluded that great apes do not exhibit understanding of human referential intentions expressed in communicative gestures, such as pointing.[29]

In birds

[edit]

Parrots

[edit]

Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) have demonstrated high levels of intelligence. Irene Pepperberg did experiments with these and her most accomplished parrot, Alex, demonstrated behaviour which seemed to manipulate the trainer, possibly indicating theory of mind.[30]

Ravens

[edit]
Ravens adjust their caching behaviour according to whether they have been watched and who was watching them.

Ravens are members of the family Corvidae and are widely regarded as having complex cognitive abilities.[31][32] Other studies indicate that ravens recall who was watching them during caching, but also know the effects of visual barriers on what competitors can and can not see, and how this affects their pilfering.[33]

Ravens have been tested for their understanding of "seeing" as a mental state in other ravens.[34] The researchers further suggested that their findings could be considered in terms of the "minimal" (as opposed to "full-blown") ToM recently suggested.[13]

Using the Knower-Guesser approach, ravens observing a human hiding food are capable of predicting the behaviour of bystander ravens that had been visible at both, none or just one of two baiting events. The visual field of the competitors was manipulated independently of the view of the test-raven.[35]

Scrub jays

[edit]
Western scrub jays may show evidence of possessing theory of mind

Scrub jays are also corvids. Western scrub jays (Aphelocoma californica) both cache food and pilfer other scrub jays' caches. They use a range of tactics to minimise the possibility that their own caches will be pilfered. One of these tactics is to remember which individual scrub jay watched them during particular caching events and adjust their re-caching behaviour accordingly.[36] One study with particularly interesting results found that only scrub jays which had themselves pilfered would re-cache when they had been observed making the initial cache.[37] This has been interpreted as the re-caching bird projecting its own experiences of pilfering intent onto those of another potential pilferer, and taking appropriate action.[8][38]

In dogs

[edit]
Dogs can use the pointing behaviour of humans to determine the location of food.

Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) show an impressive ability to use the behaviour of humans to find food and toys using behaviours such as pointing and gazing. The performance of dogs in these studies is superior to that of NHPs,[39] however, some have stated categorically that dogs do not possess a human-like ToM.[12][40]

Similarly, dogs preferentially use the behaviour of the human Knower to indicate the location of food. This is unrelated to the sex or age of the dog. In another study, 14 of 15 dogs preferred the location indicated by the Knower on the first trial, whereas chimpanzees require approximately 100 trials to reliably exhibit the preference.[39][29]

In pigs

[edit]

An experiment at the University of Bristol found that one out of ten pigs was possibly able to understand what other pigs can see. That pig observed another pig which had view of a maze in which food was being hidden, and trailed that pig through the maze to the food. The other pigs involved in the experiment did not.[41][42]

In goats

[edit]

A 2006 study found that goats exhibited intricate social behaviours indicative of high-level cognitive processes, particularly in competitive situations. The study included an experiment in which a subordinate animal was allowed to choose between food that a dominant animal could also see and food that it could not; those who were subject to aggressive behaviour selected the food that the dominant animal could not see, suggesting that they are able to perceive a threat based on being within the dominant animal's view – in other words, visual perspective taking.[43]

See also

[edit]

References

[edit]
  1. ^ a b c Heyes, C. (2015). "Animal mindreading: what's the problem?". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 22 (2): 313–327. doi:10.3758/s13423-014-0704-4. PMID 25102928. S2CID 37581900.
  2. ^ a b Premack, D.G. & Woodruff, G. (1978). "Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1 (4): 515–526. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076512.
  3. ^ Calarge, C.; Andreasen, N.C. & O'Leary, D.S. (2003). "Visualizing how one brain understands another: a PET study of theory of mind". American Journal of Psychiatry. 160 (11): 1954–1964. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1954. PMID 14594741.
  4. ^ Elgier, A.M.; Jakovcevic, A.; Mustaca, A.E. & Bentosela, M. (2012). "Pointing following in dogs: are simple or complex cognitive mechanisms involved?". Animal Cognition. 15 (6): 1111–1119. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0534-6. hdl:11336/67330. S2CID 12872446.
  5. ^ a b c Towner, S. (2010). "Concept of mind in non-human primates". Bioscience Horizons. 3 (1): 96–104. doi:10.1093/biohorizons/hzq011.
  6. ^ a b c Heyes, C.M. (1998). "Theory of mind in nonhuman primates" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 21 (1): 101–114. doi:10.1017/s0140525x98000703. PMID 10097012. S2CID 6469633.
  7. ^ a b Hare, B.; Call, J.; Agnetta, B.; Tomasello, M. (2000). "Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 59 (4): 771–785. doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1377. PMID 10792932. S2CID 3432209.
  8. ^ a b c Whiten, A. (2013). "Humans are not alone in computing how others see the world". Animal Behaviour. 86 (2): 213–221. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.04.021. S2CID 53164765.
  9. ^ a b Penn, D.C. & Povinelli, D.J. (2007). "On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a 'theory of mind'". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society. 362 (1480): 731–744. doi:10.1098/rstb.2006.2023. PMC 2346530. PMID 17264056.
  10. ^ Jabr, F. (June 8, 2010). "Clever critters: Bonobos that share, brainy bugs and social dogs". Scientific American. Retrieved April 18, 2016.
  11. ^ Horowitz, A. (2011). "Theory of mind in dogs? Examining method and concept". Learning & Behavior. 39 (4): 314–317. doi:10.3758/s13420-011-0041-7. PMID 21789555. S2CID 15883460.
  12. ^ a b Br?uer, J. (2014). "Chapter 10 – What dogs understand about humans". In Kaminski, J.; Marshall-Pescini, S. (eds.). The Social Dog: Behaviour and Cognition. Academic Press. pp. 295–317.
  13. ^ a b Butterfill, S.A. & Apperly, I.A. (2013). "How to construct a minimal theory of mind" (PDF). Mind & Language. 28 (5): 606–637. doi:10.1111/mila.12036.
  14. ^ Smith, R.A. (October 16, 2016). "Apes understand that some things are all in your head". DukeToday. Retrieved January 3, 2017.
  15. ^ Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (1998). "Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and human children (Homo sapiens)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 112 (2): 192–206. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.192. PMID 9642787.
  16. ^ Povinelli, D.J. & Vonk, J. (2003). "Chimpanzee minds: Suspiciously human?". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (4): 157–160. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.494.1478. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00053-6. PMID 12691763. S2CID 3473587.
  17. ^ Call, J.; Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (1998). "Chimpanzee gaze following in an object choice task". Animal Cognition. 1 (2): 89–99. doi:10.1007/s100710050013. PMID 24399273. S2CID 13210426.
  18. ^ Hare, B.; Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2001). "Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know and do not know?". Animal Behaviour. 61 (1): 139–151. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1518. PMID 11170704. S2CID 3402554.
  19. ^ Warneken, Felix; Tomasello, Michael (2025-08-06). "Altruistic Helping in Human Infants and Young Chimpanzees". Science. 311 (5765): 1301–1303. Bibcode:2006Sci...311.1301W. doi:10.1126/science.1121448. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 16513986. S2CID 1119115.
  20. ^ Schmelz, M.; Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2011). "Chimpanzees know that others make inferences". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA. 108 (7): 3077–3079. Bibcode:2011PNAS..108.3077S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1000469108. PMC 3041140. PMID 21282649.
  21. ^ Schmelz, M.; Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2013). "Chimpanzees predict that a competitor's preference will match their own". Biology Letters. 9 (1): 20120829. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2012.0829. PMC 3565493. PMID 23193044.
  22. ^ Melis, A.P.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. (2006). "Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) conceal visual and auditory information from others" (PDF). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 120 (2): 154–62. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.606.8937. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.120.2.154. PMID 16719594. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2025-08-06. Retrieved 2025-08-06.
  23. ^ "A Voluble Visit with Two Talking Apes". NPR.org. Retrieved 2025-08-06.
  24. ^ Clay, Zanna (2013). "Bonobos Respond to Distress in Others: Consolation across the Age Spectrum". PLOS ONE. 8 (1): e55206. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...855206C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0055206. PMC 3559394. PMID 23383110.
  25. ^ a b Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2016). "Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs". Science. 354 (6308): 110–114. Bibcode:2016Sci...354..110K. doi:10.1126/science.aaf8110. hdl:10161/13632. PMID 27846501.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  26. ^ Flombaum, J.I. & Santos, L.R. (2005). "Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others". Current Biology. 15 (5): 447–452. Bibcode:2005CBio...15..447F. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.12.076. PMID 15753039. S2CID 15978970.
  27. ^ San?Galli, A.; Devaine, M.; Trapanese, C.; Masi, S.; Bouret, S. & Daunizeau, J. (2015). "Playing hide and seek with primates: A comparative study of Theory of Mind". Revue de Primatologie. 6 (6): 4–7. doi:10.4000/primatologie.2182.
  28. ^ Herrmann, E.; Call, J.; Hernández-Lloreda, M.V.; Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (2007). "Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis". Science. 317 (5843): 1360–1366. Bibcode:2007Sci...317.1360H. doi:10.1126/science.1146282. PMID 17823346.
  29. ^ a b Topál, J.; Gergely, G.; Erd?hegyi, A.; Csibra, G. & Miklósi, A. (2009). "Differential sensitivity to human communication in dogs, wolves, and human infants". Science. 325 (5945): 1269–1272. Bibcode:2009Sci...325.1269T. doi:10.1126/science.1176960. PMID 19729660. S2CID 803577.
  30. ^ Irene Pepperberg (2012), "Theory of Mind", in Maggie Tallerman; Kathleen R. Gibson (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution, Oxford University Press, p. 114, ISBN 9780199541119
  31. ^ Emery, N.J. & Clayton, N.S. (2004). "The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes" (PDF). Science. 306 (5703): 1903–1907. Bibcode:2004Sci...306.1903E. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.299.6596. doi:10.1126/science.1098410. PMID 15591194. S2CID 9828891.
  32. ^ Bugnyar, T. & Heinrich, B. (2006). "Pilfering ravens, Corvus corax, adjust their behaviour to social context and identity of competitors". Animal Cognition. 9 (4): 369–376. doi:10.1007/s10071-006-0035-6. PMID 16909235. S2CID 1136670.
  33. ^ Bugnyar, T. & Heinrich, B. (2005). "Ravens, Corvus corax, differentiate between knowledgeable and ignorant competitors". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 272 (1573): 1641–1646. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3144. PMC 1559847. PMID 16087417.
  34. ^ Bugnyar, T.; Reber, S.A. & Buckner, C. (2016). "Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competitors". Nature Communications. 7: 10506. Bibcode:2016NatCo...710506B. doi:10.1038/ncomms10506. PMC 4740864. PMID 26835849.
  35. ^ Bugnyar, T. (2010). "Knower–guesser differentiation in ravens: others' viewpoints matter". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 278 (1705): 634–640. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1514. PMC 3025684. PMID 20826480.
  36. ^ Dally, J.M.; Emery, N.J.; Clayton, N.S. (2006). "Food-caching western scrub-jays keep track of who was watching when". Science. 312 (5780): 1662–1665. Bibcode:2006Sci...312.1662D. doi:10.1126/science.1126539. PMID 16709747. S2CID 21976318.
  37. ^ Emery, N. J. & Clayton, N.S. (2001). "Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays". Nature. 414 (6862): 443–446. Bibcode:2001Natur.414..443E. doi:10.1038/35106560. PMID 11719804. S2CID 4349432. Archived from the original on 2025-08-06. Retrieved 2025-08-06.
  38. ^ van der Vaart E.; Verbrugge R. & Hemelrijk, C.K. (2012). "Corvid re-caching without 'Theory of Mind': A model". PLOS ONE. 7 (3): e32904. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...732904V. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0032904. PMC 3291480. PMID 22396799.
  39. ^ a b Maginnity, M.E. & Grace, R.C. (2014). "Visual perspective taking by dogs (Canis familiaris) in a Guesser–Knower task: evidence for a canine theory of mind?". Animal Cognition. 17 (6): 1375–1392. doi:10.1007/s10071-014-0773-9. PMID 24950722. S2CID 14833483.
  40. ^ Kaminski, J.; Br?uer, J.; Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2009). "Domestic dogs are sensitive to a human's perspective" (PDF). Behaviour. 146 (7): 979–998. doi:10.1163/156853908X395530. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2025-08-06. Retrieved 2025-08-06.
  41. ^ Aldhous, P. (February 10, 2015). "The smartest animal you've never heard of". Wellcome Trust. Retrieved April 18, 2016.
  42. ^ Held, S.; Mendl, M.; Devereux, C.; Byrne, R.W. (2001). "Behaviour of domestic pigs in a visual perspective taking task". Behaviour. 138 (11): 1337–1354. doi:10.1163/156853901317367627.
  43. ^ Kaminski, J.; Call, J.; Tomasello, M. (2006). "Goats' behaviour in a competitive food paradigm: Evidence for perspective taking?" (PDF). Behaviour. 143 (11): 1341–1356. doi:10.1163/156853906778987542.

Further reading

[edit]
梦见捡手机是什么意思 6月25什么星座 cr是什么意思 缘分使然是什么意思 医的笔顺是什么
阿尔茨海默病吃什么药 诚字属于五行属什么 舌苔厚黄吃什么药 为什么会得脑梗 艺名是什么意思
体检吃早餐有什么影响 腿毛旺盛是什么原因 吃东西容易呛到是什么原因 胃疼是什么感觉 延时吃什么药
画什么才好看 避重就轻是什么意思 咳白色泡沫痰是什么病 宫颈多发纳囊是什么病 手黄是什么原因
肾钙化是什么意思hcv7jop6ns8r.cn 阿司匹林和阿莫西林有什么区别hcv8jop9ns9r.cn 杏不能和什么一起吃hkuteam.com 拉钩为什么要上吊hcv7jop9ns2r.cn 甲状腺结节是什么原因引起的hcv8jop6ns8r.cn
开心果为什么叫开心果hcv8jop6ns7r.cn 牙神经痛吃什么药gysmod.com 麻头是什么hcv8jop5ns0r.cn 冤亲债主是什么意思hcv8jop6ns0r.cn 什么是灰指甲aiwuzhiyu.com
黑怕是什么意思hcv8jop9ns1r.cn 狗狗流眼泪是什么原因hcv9jop4ns0r.cn 氨基酸的作用是什么hcv9jop1ns3r.cn 杏色搭配什么颜色好看wzqsfys.com 当律师需要什么条件cj623037.com
洋红色是什么颜色zhongyiyatai.com 喝什么饮料解酒最快最有效hcv8jop7ns4r.cn 吃芒果对人有什么好处hcv8jop5ns2r.cn 雾化是什么意思hcv8jop3ns7r.cn 背疼什么原因hcv7jop5ns2r.cn
百度